Which of the following theories specifically predicts that we will be more altruistic towards our relatives than towards close friends?

Abstract

This study looked at personality trait and personality disorder correlates of self-rated altruism. In two studies over 4,000 adult British managers completed a battery of tests including a ‘bright side’ personality trait measure [HPI]; a ‘dark side’/disorders measure [HDS], and a measure of their Motives and Values which included Altruism. The two studies showed similar results revealing that those who were low on Adjustment [Neuroticism] but high on Interpersonal Sensitivity [Agreeableness], Prudence [Conscientiousness] and Inquisitiveness [Openness] were more likely to value Altruism and be motivated to commit altruistic acts which concerns helping others and creating an environment that places emphasis on customer service. Those more interested in “Getting Along” with others were more Altruistic than those more interested in “Getting Ahead” of others. Implications for the selection and management of altruistic people in a business are considered. Limitations and future directions of this research are also noted.

Introduction

Altruism concerns behaviours an actor [an individual, group, or organisation] partakes in, at their own cost, in order provide benefit or increases the fitness of a recipient [Kerr et al. 2004; Oda et al. 2014]. It can be defined a selfless exhibition of trading one’s personal resources to benefit another. These traded resources can be of great importance and variety, with individuals sacrificing time, money, and organs for another [Ben-Ner and Kramer 2011].

Altruism could be defined as ethical doctrine where the moral value of an individual’s actions depend solely on the impact on other individuals, regardless of the consequences on the individual itself. Ethically, altruism is often seen as a form of consequentialism, because it indicates that an action is ethically right if it brings good consequences to others. Altruism is similar, in some senses, to formal utilitarianism, which prescribes acts that maximise good consequences for all of society, while altruism prescribes maximising good consequences for everyone other than the actor.

There are also many philosophic debates about such things as whether human conduct can ever be genuinely altruistic and indeed whether it is always desirable [Pugmire 1978]. Indeed it has also been suggested that “unfocused” corporate altruism can even be dangerous to those in need of assistance [Mitschow 2000].

It has been pointed out that often altruistic acts are promoted, and indeed made logistically possible, by organisations and institutions with a strong interest in producing them [Healy 2004]. Writers have suggested that some organisations have begun to realise that promoting altruism as a corporate goal is beneficial and may indeed enhance overall organisational effectiveness [Kanungo and Conger 1993]. Clearly, senior managers’ personal values [which may include altruism] are potential drivers of corporate social responsibility.

Hemingway and Maclagan [2004] have suggested that a manager’s corporate social responsibility can be described in two dimensions: motivational basis from altruistic to strategic and locus of responsibility from corporate to individual. They argue that individual manager’s business ethics decisions are driven by their personal values and beliefs. They exhibit these values through the exercise of personal discretion and decision making particularly the allocation of resources. They argue for an emphasis on personal initiative to counter a tendency to view the corporation as the agent of business ethics. This study looks at various individual difference correlates of those who espouse the value of altruism. Indeed there is now an interest in leaders and managers attitudes and beliefs [i.e. Machiavellianism] and their moral behaviour at work [Sendjaya et al. 2014].

Altruism can also be considered as a corporate ethical value and studies have shown that career satisfaction mediates between corporate values and personal altruism at work [Valentine et al. 2011]. However, it is quite possible that altruism could have negative consequences for an organisation particularly in family firms where decisions may be made more on the basis of biological ties than actual performance. It is quite possible that excessive altruistic citizenship behaviour over time could hurt and organisation indeed leading it to collapse.

But why do some people advocate and demonstrate altruism in their personal and social lives and others do not? This study looks at the “bright” and “dark” side correlates of self-reported altruism. By understanding the personality correlates of the altruistic individual at work it may be possible to identify those most likely to make unethical, immoral or selfish ethical decisions in corporate settings. The idea is that traits being more stable are the part drivers of values rather than the other way around. Correlational results do not provide evidence of causality though it is more likely that stable, biologically based traits shape motives and values which are less stable and open to modification and change [Furnham 2008].

In the literature, altruistic behaviours are either measured through an individual’s self-reported likelihood of partaking in various scenarios [e.g. ‘I have given money to charity’; Rushton et al. [1981], or recording actual behavioural responses [e.g. money donated to the other player in the dictator game [Camerer 2003]]. Others have looked at the extent to which people say they value and are personally motivated to be altruistic. However, the results on personality correlates of different sorts of altruistic behaviours are not consistent from different studies. Bekkers [2006] found that high Openness and Extraversion led to more frequent charitable donation, whilst high Agreeableness was related to more frequent blood donation. Extraversion has been thought to influence altruism due to a desire for engaging with other individuals. Studies have indicated a moderate-to-weak, positive correlation between Extraversion and altruism [Ben-Ner and Kramer 2011; Oda et al. 2014]. Swope et al. [2008], however, found no relation between personality correlates and amount given in an altruism game.

In a more recent study using the HEXACO model of personality, Aghababaei et al. [2014] found their prosocial personality test which is clearly a measure of altruism to be significantly correlated with all six major personality factors: Honesty–Humility, r = .51; Agreeableness, r = .43; Conscientiousness, r = .38, Openness, r = .24; Emotionality, r = .22 and Extraversion r = .19. Interestingly, the Dark Triad trait composite of Machiavellianism, narcissism and psychopathy was not a significant [negative] predictor as expected.

‘Dark’ side personality traits which are a manifestation of the personality disorders are also related to the probability of engaging in altruism. Thus, primary psychopathy and callousness have been shown to be uniquely and inversely related to altruism, whilst secondary psychopathy [when mediated by empathy] was inversely associated with altruism [White 2014].

Personality correlates seem to be more significant in explaining altruistic behaviours when the relationship between the actor and recipient is considered. Evolutionary perspectives outline specific drivers for different altruistic relationships. Altruism towards one’s kin is motivated by the corollary of greatly increasing the chance of genetic survival, whilst non-relative altruism is an investment in the recipient to return the act in the future. Familial altruism fosters a ‘kinship premium’ [Curry et al. 2013], where altruism levels towards kin compared to non-relatives are significantly higher, even when social distance and emotional closeness are controlled for [Curry et al. 2013; Rachlin and Jones 2008]. This issue is particularly relevant in family businesses [Boyd et al. 2013].

By investigating differing types of actor-recipient relationship, personality correlates of altruism become apparent. Ashton et al. [1998] found kin altruism to be facilitated by Emotional/Attachment personality constructs, which correlate significantly with high Agreeableness and low Emotional Stability. Non-relative/reciprocal altruism, however, has been associated with Forgiveness/Non-Retaliation constructs, which are correlated with high Agreeableness and Emotional Stability. In altruism towards ‘collaborators’ and ‘competitors’, Neuroticism has been found to positively affect giving, whilst a concave/U-curve [declining first, then increasing at higher levels] relationship was noted for Extraversion and Conscientiousness [Ben-Ner and Kramer 2011]. Extraversion and Openness have been found to predict increased daily altruism towards friends and acquaintances, as well as strangers [Oda et al. 2014]. Some research, however, found personality to play no role in giving to a kin, a result “consistent with the evolutionary view, that kin altruism is hard wired and strong, and therefore few individual differences will be observed in it.” [Ben-Ner and Kramer 2011, p. 221].

This Study

In this study, we used the altruism scale from the motives, values, preferences inventory [MVPI] as the criterion [Hogan et al. 2007]. The scale measures a desire to help others, a concern for the welfare of the less fortunate in life, and a lifestyle organised around public service and the betterment of humanity. According to the manual high scorers are perceived as honest, ethical, sympathetic and concerned about others. They have a need for nurturance which focuses on helping, protecting and caring for those in need in the workplace and other social settings. At work, those with altruistic motives appear to enjoy helping others, promoting staff morale, fostering open communications and trying to help their subordinates achieve greater engagement and advancement. On the other hand, low scorers are perceived as tough, uncommunicative and materialistic. According to the manual they tend to be assertive, confrontational, direct and outspoken [Hogan et al. 2007]. Clearly, the more senior and powerful an individual at work is, the more influence they can exercise and the more important their values in establishing and maintaining corporate ethics [Hemingway and Maclagan 2004].

In this study, we looked a personality correlates of altruistic values. The “bright side” traits were measured using the Hogan Personality Inventory [Hogan et al. 2007]. The HPI consists of 206 items that are used to produce seven personality traits and six criterion scores. The seven personality traits are: Adjustment [Neuroticism]; Ambition [Leadership and Status Seeking]; Sociability [Extraversion]; Interpersonal Sensitivity: [Agreeableness]; Prudence [Conscientiousness]; Inquisitive [Openness]; Learning Approach [Need for Intellectual Stimulation] [Hogan et al. 2007]. Based on the previous literature, we predict [H1] Interpersonal Sensitivity which is essentially a measure of Agreeableness would be most strongly and positively correlated with altruism. This is based on the findings of Aghababaei et al. [2014] and Ashton et al. [1998].

This study used the Hogan Developmental Survey now extensively used in organisational research and practice to measure “dark side” personality disorders in the ‘normal population’. The HDS focuses only on the core construct of each disorder from a dimensional perspective [Hogan and Hogan 2001, p. 41]. Various relatively studies have used the HDS and have shown it to be a robust, reliable and valid instrument [Furnham and Trickey 2011; Furnham et al. 2013]. It should be noted that these personality disorders are grouped along different axes or different clusters. When clustering three are usually made: A: Odd/Eccentric [Paranoid, Schizoid, Schizotypal]; B: Dramatic/Emotional/Erratic [Antisocial, Borderline, Histrionic, Narcissistic] and C: Anxious/Fearful [Avoidant, Dependent and Obsessive–Compulsive]. These three clusters have also been described as Moving Against Others [by deliberately manipulating and controlling others] Moving Toward Others [by building alliances with others] and Moving Away From others [by maintaining their distance and pushing others away [Hogan et al. 2007].

There is little data or theorising in this area to formulate hypotheses though it was predicted that the Moving Away Traits: Excitable [H2] Sceptical [H3], and Reserved [H3] would be negatively correlated with Altruism, while the Moving Toward traits, Diligent [H4] and Dutiful [H5] would be positively related to Altruism. This is based on the work of Furnham et al. [2014] who showed that people who had this profile were attracted to more traditional jobs and institutions which upheld high moral and pro-social values.

Method

Participants

Study 1 There were a total of 1,458 participants of whom 874 were males and 584 females. Their mean age was 36.14 years [SD = 12.90 years]. They were all middle to senior managers of various British and European organisations.

Study 2 There were a total of 2,548 participants of whom 1,744 were males and 884 females. Their mean age was 40.16 years [SD = 16.33 years]. They were also all middle to senior managers assessed for selection, development and promotion.

Measures

  1. 1.

    Values The MVPI [Hogan et al. 2007] measures 10 Motives/Preferences. Each scale is composed of five themes: [a] lifestyles, which concern the manner in which a person would like to live, [b] beliefs, which involve ‘shoulds’, ideals and ultimate life goals, [c] occupational Preferences, which include the work an individual would like to do, what constitutes a good job, and preferred work materials, [d] aversions, which reflect attitudes and behaviours that are either disliked or distressing, and [e] preferred Associates, which include the kind of persons desired as co-workers and friends. MVPI scores are quite stable over time, with test–retest reliabilities ranging between .64 and .88 [mean = .79]. More than 100 validation studies have been conducted on the MVPI with results indicating that the inventory is effective in predicting job performance and outcome variables such as turnover [Hogan Assessment Systems; Tulsa, USA]. In this study, we focus exclusively on the value of altruism.

  2. 2.

    Dark Side Traits Hogan Development Survey [Hogan et al. 2007] is a measure of the personality disorders expressed in non-clinical language. The survey includes 154 items, scored for 11 scales, each grouping 14 items. Respondents are requested to ‘agree’ or ‘disagree’ with the items. The HDS has been cross-validated with the MMPI personality disorder scales. It has considerable evidence of satisfactory reliability and validity [Fico et al. 2000; Hogan and Hogan 2001]. Furnham and Crump [2005] show the overlap of the HDS and DSM-IV disorder terminology. There are good British norms for this measure [Furnham and Trickey 2011] and it has been used in various recent studies [Furnham et al. 2012, 2013, 2014].

  3. 3.

    Personality Hogan Personality Inventory [Hogan et al. 2007] is a measure of normal personality functioning closely aligned to the Big Five. It measures seven dimensions of personality adjustment, ambition, sociability, interpersonal sensitivity, prudence, inquisitive and learning approach. It was initially developed in 1976. It has 7 domains and 41 facets measured by 206 items. It has impressive evidence of reliability and validity and used in many studies [Hogan et al. 2007].

Procedure

Data for study 1 and study 2 were obtained from two consultancies. In the first study, participants completed the tests online before attending either training or coaching sessions where they were given full feedback on their test results. In the second study, participants were required to attend a middle management assessment centre where they completed the questionnaires. The assessment was aimed at determining the suitability of each manager for promotion. Each manager was given feedback on the results, including how he/she related to the test norms as well as his/her colleagues.

Results

Correlations and Factor Analyses

Table 1 shows the correlational results between Altruism and the seven Bright Side factors for both studies. The results were consistent and indicated that five of the seven factors were significantly positively correlated to Altruism particularly Interpersonal Sensitivity [also known as Agreeableness]. Factor analysis also confirmed that for both analyses a two factor model emerged identical to Digman’s [1997] Alpha [Communion] and Beta [Agency] model where the former comprises Agreeableness [Interpersonal Sensitivity], Conscientiousness [Prudence] and Neuroticism [Adjustment], and the latter Extraversion [Ambition and Sociability] and Openness [Inquisitive and Learning Approach].

Table 1 Means, SDs, factor analytic results, and correlations between HPI scales and altruism for both studies

Full size table

Table 2 shows the correlational results for Altruism and the eleven Dark Side factors for both studies. The results were reasonably consistent and indicated that three scales [Excitable, Sceptical, and Reserved] were negatively, and four scales [Colourful, Imaginative, Diligent, Dutiful] positively, correlated with Altruism. Factor analysis of the eleven factors yielded the well-replicated three-fold structure entitled Moving Away, Moving Against and Moving Toward others.

Table 2 Means, SDs, factor analytic results, and correlations between HDS scales and attribution for both studies

Full size table

Regressions

Table 3 shows the results of two identical regressions for sample 1 and 2. For both, the criterion variable was the Altruism score. In a step-wise regression first age and sex were entered, then social desirability, then the seven Bright Side factors. Results were similar for both analyses. Older females tended to have higher scores and those factors accounted for 3–4 % of the variance. The consistent findings for the bright side traits indicated the more highly people score on Interpersonal Sensitivity, Inquisitiveness and Prudence, and the lower they score on Adjustment, the more altruistic they were. Personality factors accounted for around 17 % of the variance.

Table 3 Results for the regression using the HPI for both studies

Full size table

This regression was repeated but this time using the “higher-order” alpha–beta model. Study 1 was significant [F[5,1452] = 39.05, p 

Bài Viết Liên Quan

Chủ Đề