Health behavior change can occur in response to messages outside ones awareness

The Health Belief Model is a theoretical model that can be used to guide health promotion and disease prevention programs. It is used to explain and predict individual changes in health behaviors. It is one of the most widely used models for understanding health behaviors.

Key elements of the Health Belief Model focus on individual beliefs about health conditions, which predict individual health-related behaviors. The model defines the key factors that influence health behaviors as an individual's perceived threat to sickness or disease (perceived susceptibility), belief of consequence (perceived severity), potential positive benefits of action (perceived benefits), perceived barriers to action, exposure to factors that prompt action (cues to action), and confidence in ability to succeed (self-efficacy).

Health Belief Model Examples

  • The Michigan Model for Health™ is a curriculum designed for implementation in schools. It targets social and emotional health challenges including nutrition, physical activity, alcohol and drug use, safety, and personal health, among other topics. This model adapts components of the Health Belief Model related to knowledge, skills, self-efficacy, and environmental support.

Considerations for Implementation

The Health Belief Model can be used to design short- and long-term interventions. The five key action-related components that determine the ability of the Health Belief Model to identify key decision-making points that influence health behaviors are:

  • Gathering information by conducting a health needs assessments and other efforts to determine who is at risk and the population(s) that should be targeted.
  • Conveying the consequences of the health issues associated with risk behaviors in a clear and unambiguous fashion to understand perceived severity.
  • Communicating to the target population the steps that are involved in taking the recommended action and highlighting the benefits to action.
  • Providing assistance in identifying and reducing barriers to action.
  • Demonstrating actions through skill development activities and providing support that enhances self-efficacy and the likelihood of successful behavior changes.

These actions represent key elements of the Health Belief Model and can be used to design or adapt health promotion or disease prevention programs. The Health Belief Model is appropriate to be used alone or in combination with other theories or models. To ensure success with this model, it is important to identify "cues to action" that are meaningful and appropriate for the target population.

We suggest that six common errors made by policy makers prevent the successful implementation of health-related behaviour change.

We argue that the extensive body of evidence of how to bring abut behaviour change is consistently ignored.

The automatic and reflective systems described in psychology and social practice theory described in sociology are particularly important new areas for developing ideas about behaviour change.

Abstract

Objective

To demonstrate that six common errors made in attempts to change behaviour have prevented the implementation of the scientific evidence base derived from psychology and sociology; to suggest a new approach which incorporates recent developments in the behavioural sciences.

Study design

The role of health behaviours in the origin of the current epidemic of non-communicable disease is observed to have driven attempts to change behaviour. It is noted that most efforts to change health behaviours have had limited success. This paper suggests that in policy-making, discussions about behaviour change are subject to six common errors and that these errors have made the business of health-related behaviour change much more difficult than it needs to be.

Methods

Overview of policy and practice attempts to change health-related behaviour.

Results

The reasons why knowledge and learning about behaviour have made so little progress in alcohol, dietary and physical inactivity-related disease prevention are considered, and an alternative way of thinking about the behaviours involved is suggested. This model harnesses recent developments in the behavioural sciences.

Conclusion

It is important to understand the conditions preceding behaviour psychologically and sociologically and to combine psychological ideas about the automatic and reflective systems with sociological ideas about social practice.

Risk perceptions – or an individual’s perceived susceptibility to a threat – are a key component of many health behavior change theories. Risk perceptions are often targeted in health behavior change interventions, and recent meta-analytic evidence suggests that interventions that successfully engage and change risk perceptions produce subsequent increases in health behaviors. Here, we review recent literature on risk perceptions and health behavior, including research on the formation of risk perceptions, types of risk perceptions (including deliberative, affective, and experiential), accuracy of risk perceptions, and associations and interactions among types of risk perceptions. Taken together, existing research suggests that disease risk perceptions are a critical determinant of health behavior, although the nature of the association among risk perceptions and health behavior may depend on the profile of different types of risk perceptions and the accuracy of such perceptions.

In health decision-making, individuals are expected to navigate choices involving weighing risk for consequences with benefits of action. Behaviors contributing to disease initiation and progression are often pleasurable (e.g., smoking or overeating). Motivation to forgo such pleasurable behaviors, or engage in inconvenient preventive behaviors, is believed to be driven to some extent by beliefs about the probability that a health consequence will occur [1-2]. Correlational evidence supports an at-least-modest association between risk perceptions and health behaviors [3-4].

Theory-guided health behavior change interventions and health communications often target risk perceptions toward the end of changing health behaviors [5]. A recent meta-analysis of experimental evidence supports the role of risk perceptions in health decision-making; when interventions successfully change risk perceptions, health behavior change often results [6]. Risk perceptions may also have implications for overall well-being as threats unfold. For example, prospective evidence demonstrates that, among individuals with high cancer risk perceptions, subsequent cancer diagnosis is associated with poorer well-being; however, among those with low cancer risk perceptions, subsequent cancer diagnosis is unrelated to well-being [7].

Formation of risk perceptions

A growing body of literature has probed how risk perceptions are formed. Although risk perceptions can be optimistic (i.e., low) or pessimistic (i.e., high), they are empirically and conceptually distinct from general dispositional optimism, in part because they are domain-specific [8]. Indeed, evidence suggests that, in the general population, individuals are able to differentiate among specific threats when forming risk perceptions [9]. Moreover, several studies suggest that dispositional and domain-specific optimism may interact in ways with important implications for health [10]. For example, individuals high in dispositional optimism who also have optimistic risk perceptions regarding a looming threat may be more likely to minimize the threat’s severity and less likely to seek additional health information [11].

Given that risk perceptions involve incorporating numeric information about a threat, the ability to produce, understand, and use numeric information plays an important role in the formation and use of risk perceptions [12]. Indeed, evidence shows that individuals who are highly numerate are more likely to retrieve and use numerical principles in decision-making, rendering them less susceptible to biases related to risk perception and decision-making, and less likely to incorporate irrelevant information into risk perceptions [13]. However, evidence suggests that risk perceptions are reflective of not only numeric information, but also information regarding personal experiences. For example, enactment of precautionary behavior results in subsequent, appropriate reductions in risk perception [14], and engaging in risky behaviors is associated with appropriately higher risk perceptions [15]. Moreover, risk perceptions are influenced by what information is most salient or available to an individual [16]. For example, individuals perceive their risk for disease to be higher when someone in their family has been diagnosed with a disease [17]. Although factors like family history arguably provide some relevant information about actual susceptibility to disease, other salient information also plays a role in risk perception formation. For example, risk perceptions are often influenced by the frequency with which a threat is represented in media exposure [18].

Risk perceptions are also reliably influenced by contextual factors. For example, as looming threats become more immediate, risk perceptions tend to become more pessimistic [19]. Risk perceptions also tend to be higher when a health threat is seen as uncontrollable or dreaded [18]. Moreover, affective contextual factors play a critical role; individuals experiencing anger (a high certainty and control emotion) tend to have more optimistic risk perceptions, whereas those experiencing fear (a low certainty and control emotion) tend to have more pessimistic risk perceptions [20]. General affect can also influence the formation of risk perceptions. For example, distress is associated with higher risk perceptions [21], and depressed individuals may be more likely to adjust their risk perception estimates in response to health information than non-depressed individuals [22]. These tendencies have important implications for the formation of risk perceptions in a health context, particularly given that many health threats and clinical care contexts evoke strong emotions [23].

In sum, risk perceptions are threat-specific, rather than reflecting a general sense of optimism or pessimism. Although risk perceptions incorporate numeric information, a number of additional factors contribute to their formation, including personal experiences, salience of available examples, and affective factors.

Types of risk perceptions

Classic health behavior theories largely treat risk perceptions as deliberatively-derived judgments, and research synthesized thus far has fit this conceptualization. Deliberative risk perceptions are systematic, logical, and rule-based [24, 25]. Theories that emphasize deliberative risk perceptions suggest that an individual relies on a number of reason-based strategies to derive an estimate of the likelihood that the negative outcome will occur. Deliberative risk perceptions are usually absolute (e.g., percentage likelihood of disease) or comparative (e.g., likelihood of disease compared to others).

However, recent models of risk perception and decision-making have highlighted the divide between 1) deliberative and 2) affective or experiential components [26-28]. Affective risk perceptions refer to affect associated with risk. Affect has been established as an essential determinant of optimal judgment and decision-making [29], and is a critical component of judgments involving risk and uncertainty [27]. Worry or anxiety about a threat is considered to be an affective analogue to deliberative risk perceptions [27]. Meta-analytic evidence demonstrates that affective risk perceptions are related to preventive behaviors [30], and that interventions that successfully target these perceptions produce subsequent changes in behavior [6].

Experiential risk perceptions refer to rapid judgments made by integrating deliberative and affective information [31-32]. Consistent with existing terminology and theory [33], experiential risk perceptions refer to the contents of the perception as opposed to the process through which the perception is derived; thus, they are by definition consciously accessible. For example, an individual is consciously aware that her intuition or “gut” is telling her she is vulnerable to cancer, even if she has no conscious access to the processes that contributed to the formation of that judgment. Examples of experiential risk perceptions include gut-level assessments of vulnerability (e.g., “how vulnerable do you feel?” [34] or gist-representations of risk [35]. Experiential risk perceptions are often more predictive of intentions or behavior than are deliberative risk perceptions [34, 36].

Critically, existing frameworks tend to combine or conflate affective and experiential components, or focus on one over the other as the non-deliberative component [26, 28, 37]. However, evidence suggests these are empirically distinct not only from deliberative components but also from one another [38-41].Thus, a more fine-grained and accurate distinction among these three types of risk perception – deliberative, affective, and experiential – can improve the predictive value of existing and emerging frameworks, and help applied researchers and practitioners to more effectively target the active ingredients necessary to facilitate behavior change.

Accuracy of risk perceptions

The formation of accurate – or inaccurate – risk perceptions may have important consequences for health. Although low risk perceptions are by definition optimistic, if an individual is indeed at low risk for a disease threat, those risk perceptions are also realistic. However, often individuals believe themselves to be at lower risk for outcomes than is warranted when examining their objective risk; this phenomenon is termed “unrealistic optimism” [42]. Note that accuracy of risk perceptions depends on measurement; an individual’s risk perceptions regarding the same disease can be simultaneously pessimistic and optimistic when assessed with absolute and comparative measures, respectively [43]. For example, a woman with objectively high risk of breast cancer can estimate she has a 70% chance of breast cancer (an unrealistically pessimistic absolute estimate), but simultaneously report she is at lower risk than other women her age (an unrealistically optimistic comparative estimate). Unrealistic optimism, particularly as a comparative assessment, is quite prevalent in the general population [44].

Evidence regarding the implications of unrealistic optimism is mixed. Some studies suggest that unrealistic optimism yields lower motivation to engage in health protective behaviors that would mitigate risk [45-46], and unrealistic optimism has been linked to objective negative health outcomes [47-48]. However, other studies have linked unrealistic optimism to positive health outcomes [49-51]. Despite mixed evidence regarding implications, the extant literature clearly suggests that risk perceptions can be unrealistically optimistic, and that this is a fairly common bias.

Associations and interactions among types of risk perceptions

Importantly, existing models do not directly address the possibility of a more complex interplay between deliberative and affective influences, despite the fact that evidence suggests that the strength of the associations among deliberative and affective components of risk perceptions may be as important as the absolute magnitudes of those constructs. For example, choice preference strength and readiness for action may be strongest among individuals when deliberative and affective perceptions are in convergence [40]. These data suggest the possibility that a coherent risk perception schema, demonstrated by logical associations between the deliberative and affective risk perceptions, may be just as or more important than the absolute level of risk perceptions and worry.

Complex interactions between affective and deliberative risk perception components are also important to consider. There may be a combination of risk perceptions that could result in optimal - or non-optimal - decisions. Research suggests that deliberative and affective risk perception components may indeed interact in this way, such that individuals who are worried about an outcome and perceive themselves to be at high risk for that outcome are less motivated or less likely to engage in preventive or mitigating behaviors. For example, data from nationally representative surveys of U.S. adults indicate that those reporting both high risk perceptions and high worry were significantly less likely to engage in any exercise or meet the 5-a-day fruit and vegetable consumption guidelines [52], and were more likely to report avoiding visiting their healthcare provider even when they believe they should [53]. Although these data are cross-sectional, longitudinal data also support this pattern: in the context of a smoking cessation intervention, and using a longitudinal design, data suggest that high risk perceptions and worry contribute to lower intentions to quit smoking among adults [54].

This pattern may emerge because high levels of affective and deliberative risk perceptions, in combination, activate specific experiential perceptions related to fatalistic beliefs about disease risk. However, as a caution against suggesting that the association between affective and deliberative risk perceptions is simple, we note that the pattern of this interaction is not always consistent [54-55], emphasizing the importance of leveraging a risk perception framework that could guide research to identify conditions under which, and populations for whom, the interaction occurs. In sum, although more research is necessary, empirical evidence suggests that disentangling deliberative, affective, and experiential components of risk perception is insufficient: interactions and associations among these components are critical to consider to maximize the predictive validity of these constructs and the efficacy of health behavior change interventions they inform.

Concluding remarks

Health-related risk perceptions play an important role in motivating health behavior change [6], and empirical evidence suggests that there are three distinct types of risk perceptions: deliberative, affective, and intuitive [38-41] . Much is known about the formation of deliberative health-related risk perceptions, including the role of numeracy, previous experiences and salient instances of the threat, and emotion. Moreover, research has examined the implications of accurate – and inaccurate – deliberative risk perceptions in health behaviors and outcomes. However, a dearth of research addresses the formation of affective and experiential health-related risk perceptions, and no research conceptualizes unrealistic optimism about risk perceptions using these non-deliberative judgments. Thus, future research is needed to further elucidate these topics, as well as to examine how deliberative, affective, and experiential risk perceptions interact to produce health behavior and health behavior change.

Highlights

  • Interventions that change risk perceptions subsequently change health behaviors

  • Individuals form risk perceptions tailored to specific health threats

  • Risk perceptions can refer to deliberative, affective, and experiential components

  • These components can interactively influence health behaviors.

  • The formation of accurate risk perceptions has implications for health behaviors

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

What are the 4 perceptions that influence health behaviors?

The model defines the key factors that influence health behaviors as an individual's perceived threat to sickness or disease (perceived susceptibility), belief of consequence (perceived severity), potential positive benefits of action (perceived benefits), perceived barriers to action, exposure to factors that prompt ...

What are behavioral changes examples?

What Is Behavioral Change?.
Smoking cessation..
Reducing alcohol intake..
Eating healthily..
Exercising regularly..
Practicing safe sex..
Driving safely..

What is health behavior change theory?

The HBM derives from psychological and behavioral theory with the foundation that the two components of health-related behavior are 1) the desire to avoid illness, or conversely get well if already ill; and, 2) the belief that a specific health action will prevent, or cure, illness.

How does the Health Belief Model change behaviour?

The Health Belief Model states that people's beliefs influence their health-related actions or behaviors. Individuals will likely take action when experiencing a personal threat or risk, but only if the benefits of taking action outweigh the barriers, whether real or perceived.