How does cognitive neuroscience differ from cognitive psychology?

Manipulation techniques, in contrast, examine how perturbations of the brain’s function – either by transiently changing neuronal firing rates or neurotransmitter levels or by permanently damaging tissue – change cognitive functions or behavior. Accordingly, manipulation techniques are sometimes called causal approaches. Neuroeconomists have used manipulation techniques to disrupt processing in specific regions, which in turn alters the choices people make (e.g., in interactive games).

This chapter follows this basic division, first introducing techniques that measure changes in brain function which track the variables within decision models, then considering techniques that change neural processing and also decision behavior. It is important to recognize that measurement and manipulation techniques provide distinct and complementary information about brain function. Cognitive neuroscience research progresses more quickly when measurement techniques establish links between brain structure and cognitive function and then manipulation techniques probe that relationship to improve inferences and models.

View chapterPurchase book

Read full chapter

URL: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780124160088000061

Neurotechnologies☆

A. Kayser, M. D'Esposito, in Reference Module in Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Psychology, 2017

Introduction

Cognitive neuroscience is a discipline that attempts to determine the neural mechanisms underlying cognitive processes. Specifically, cognitive neuroscientists test hypotheses about brain-behavior relationships that can be organized along two conceptual domains: functional specialization—the idea that areas of the cerebral cortex represent functional modules that are specialized for a specific cognitive process—and functional integration—the idea that a cognitive process can be an emergent property of interactions among a network of brain regions, and thus that a brain region can play a different role across many functions.

Early investigations of brain–behavior relationships consisted of careful observation of individuals with neurological injury resulting in focal brain damage. The idea of functional specialization evolved from hypotheses that damage to a particular brain region was responsible for a given behavioral syndrome characterized by a precise neurological examination. For instance, the association of nonfluent aphasia with right-sided limb weakness implicated the left hemisphere as the site of language abilities. Moreover, upon the death of a patient with a neurological disorder, clinicopathological correlations provided information confirming the site of damage that caused a specific neurobehavioral syndrome. For example, in 1861 Paul Broca's observations of nonfluent aphasia in the setting of a damaged left inferior frontal gyrus cemented the belief that this brain region was critical for speech output. The introduction of structural brain imaging more than 100 years after Broca's observations, first with computerized tomography and later with magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), paved the way for more precise anatomical localization in the living patient of the cognitive deficits that develop after brain injury. The superb spatial resolution of structural neuroimaging has reduced the reliance on the infrequently obtained autopsy for making brain–behavior correlations.

Functional neuroimaging methodologies, broadly defined as techniques that measure brain activity, have expanded our ability to study the neural basis of cognitive processes. As technology has advanced, a number of these techniques have arisen, including single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT), positron emission tomography (PET), functional MRI (fMRI), and magnetoencephalography (MEG). Using these techniques, researchers can measure regional brain activity in healthy subjects while they perform cognitive tasks, and thereby link localized brain activity with specific behaviors. For example, functional neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that the left inferior frontal gyrus is consistently activated during the performance of speech production tasks in healthy individuals. Such findings from functional neuroimaging complement findings derived from observations of patients with focal brain damage. In order to provide the reader with the necessary background for understanding functional neuroimaging data, this article focuses on the principles underlying each of these techniques, as well as their relative strengths and weaknesses. Particular attention will be paid to fMRI, as it is perhaps the most widely-employed neuroimaging method. The article concludes with some of the novel ways in which these techniques are being combined in order to harness their complementary strengths to answer questions in cognitive neuroscience.

View chapterPurchase book

Read full chapter

URL: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780128093245065007

Predictive Analytics

Colleen McCue, in Data Mining and Predictive Analysis, 2007

7.6 Unsupervised Learning Algorithms

Unsupervised learning algorithms are used to group cases based on similar attributes. These models also are referred to as self-organizing maps. Unsupervised models include clustering techniques and neural networks. Different algorithms use different strategies for dividing data into groups. Some methods are relatively straightforward, quickly dividing the cases into groups based on common attributes or some other similarity. The Two-Step clustering method differs somewhat in that an optimal number of clusters is determined in an initial pass through the data, based on certain statistical criteria. Group assignment is then made on a second pass through the data; hence the name “Two-Step.” Neural networks are more complicated than some of the other unsupervised learning algorithms and can yield results that are difficult to interpret.

Cognitive Neuroscience and Neural Nets

“My religion consists of a humble admiration of the illimitable superior spirit who reveals himself in the slight details we are able to perceive with our frail and feeble mind.”

Albert Einstein

For as long as I can remember, I have been fascinated by science and the wonders of the universe. From stargazing in the backyard with the homemade telescope that my father and I built to the absolute awe that I experience when contemplating the vastness of the cosmos and the subtle elegance of nature, I have been hooked on science from the start.

During college I began to focus my interest on neuroscience and the brain. What an incredible machine! As I sit here now I can recall the muffled quiet of my first snowfall at Dartmouth, the sound of a lawnmower running on a Saturday morning from my childhood in Downers Grove, Illinois, and the smell of fresh cut grass. I can see the windows steam up in our kitchen on Thanksgiving, and smell my mother's turkey, which I never have been able to replicate. The truly amazing thing about all of this, though, is that all of these memories, including their associated sights, smells, and sounds, reside in a mass of biological material sitting between my ears that basically has two settings: on and off. Some might argue that neuromodulators and other similar entities complicate the situation somewhat, but the bottom line is that neurons, the basic components of our brains, are either on or they are off. Like a computer, it is this combination of “on” and “off,” the interconnectedness of these simple elements and the associated parallel processing, that gives us the complexity of what we know to be brain function.

While I do not necessarily hold the conviction with Descartes that the seat of my soul resides somewhere at the base of my brain, I do know that everything from unconscious activities like breathing to my preference for the color green sits up there with rarely a conscious thought from me. More to the point, I know that the individual differences that make the world so interesting, as well as the similarities both between and within humans and their behavior that allow me to do my job as a behavioral scientist, also reside in this neural computer.

Analysts spend a considerable amount of time trying to categorize and model the complexities of human behavior. This practice is complicated even further for crime analysts because the behavior being modeled differs in some way from “normal” behavior, if only for the reason that it is illegal. In additional, criminal behavior tends to be relatively infrequent and is something that most folks have limited experience with outside of the public safety and security worlds. The ability to reduce these behaviors to patterns and trends that can be not only described but even anticipated or predicted in some situations still amazes me because it says as much about human nature as it does about analysis. In many ways, predictive analytics and artificial intelligence are fascinating in their power and complexity, but perhaps the real wonder is the fact that human behavior can even be modeled and predicted at all.

The most incomprehensible thing about the world is that it is comprehensible.

Albert Einstein

View chapterPurchase book

Read full chapter

URL: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780750677967500295

Cognitive psychology is the science in charge of understanding the main processes (mind) that help us to understand our world and neuroscience studies how the brain correlates these processes.

What is neuroscience and cognitive psychology?

Neuroscience studies the brain's structure and what areas get activated when an individual does certain tasks. Cognitive psychology looks at behaviour. Changes in the brain may or may not impact behaviour. Neuroscience at best is helping to confirm what cognitive psychology has produced in behaviour.

How is cognitive psychology different from neuropsychology?

Neuropsychology is the study of the way specific areas of the brain affect personality, behavior, and self image. Cognitive psychology is the study of the way emotional and rational motivations drive decision making and behavior.